期刊论文详细信息
BMC Evolutionary Biology
Motion dazzle and the effects of target patterning on capture success
Martin Stevens1  Jolyon Troscianko1  Anna E Hughes2 
[1] Centre for Ecology and Conservation, University of Exeter, Penryn Campus, Penryn, Cornwall TR10 9FE, UK;Department of Physiology, Development, and Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge CB2 3EG, UK
关键词: Animal coloration;    Vision;    Motion dazzle;   
Others  :  1117953
DOI  :  10.1186/s12862-014-0201-4
 received in 2014-06-05, accepted in 2014-09-09,  发布年份 2014
PDF
【 摘 要 】

Background

Stripes and other high contrast patterns found on animals have been hypothesised to cause “motion dazzle”, a type of defensive coloration that operates when in motion, causing predators to misjudge the speed and direction of object movement. Several recent studies have found some support for this idea, but little is currently understood about the mechanisms underlying this effect. Using humans as model ‘predators’ in a touch screen experiment we investigated further the effectiveness of striped targets in preventing capture, and considered how stripes compare to other types of patterning in order to understand what aspects of target patterning are important in making a target difficult to capture.

Results

We find that striped targets are among the most difficult to capture, but that other patterning types are also highly effective at preventing capture in this task. Several target types, including background sampled targets and targets with a ‘spot’ on were significantly easier to capture than striped targets. We also show differences in capture attempt rates between different target types, but we find no differences in learning rates between target types.

Conclusions

We conclude that striped targets are effective in preventing capture, but are not uniquely difficult to catch, with luminance matched grey targets also showing a similar capture rate. We show that key factors in making capture easier are a lack of average background luminance matching and having trackable ‘features’ on the target body. We also find that striped patterns are attempted relatively quickly, despite being difficult to catch. We discuss these findings in relation to the motion dazzle hypothesis and how capture rates may be affected more generally by pattern type.

【 授权许可】

   
2014 Hughes et al.; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.

【 预 览 】
附件列表
Files Size Format View
20150206013808959.pdf 693KB PDF download
Figure 3. 21KB Image download
Figure 2. 18KB Image download
Figure 1. 46KB Image download
【 图 表 】

Figure 1.

Figure 2.

Figure 3.

【 参考文献 】
  • [1]Stevens M, Merilaita S: Animal camouflage: current issues and new perspectives. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 2009, 364:423-427.
  • [2]Stevens M: Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of protective coloration. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2007, 274:1457-1464.
  • [3]Cuthill IC, Stevens M, Sheppard J, Maddocks T, Párraga CA, Troscianko TS: Disruptive coloration and background pattern matching. Nature 2005, 434:72-74.
  • [4]Stevens M, Marshall KLA, Troscianko J, Finlay S, Burnand D, Chadwick SL: Revealed by conspicuousness: distractive markings reduce camouflage. Behav Ecol 2013, 24:213-222.
  • [5]Rowland HM, Speed MP, Ruxton GD, Edmunds M, Stevens M, Harvey IF: Countershading enhances cryptic protection: an experiment with wild birds and artificial prey. Anim Behav 2007, 74:1249-1258.
  • [6]Rowland HM, Cuthill IC, Harvey IF, Speed MP, Ruxton GD: Can’t tell the caterpillars from the trees: countershading enhances survival in a woodland. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2008, 275:2539-2545.
  • [7]Fraser S, Callahan A, Klassen D, Sherratt TN: Empirical tests of the role of disruptive coloration in reducing detectability. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2007, 274:1325-1331.
  • [8]Schaefer HM, Stobbe N: Disruptive coloration provides camouflage independent of background matching. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2006, 273:2427-2432.
  • [9]Webster RJ, Hassall C, Herdman CM, Godin J-GJ, Sherratt TN: Disruptive camouflage impairs object recognition. Biol Lett 2013, 9:20130501.
  • [10]Behrens RR: The role of artists in ship camouflage during World War I. Leonardo 1999, 32:53-59.
  • [11]Forbes P: Dazzled and Deceived: Mimicry and Camouflage. Yale University Press, New Haven; 2011.
  • [12]Shine R, Madsen T: Sexual dischromatism in snakes of the genus vipera: a review and a new evolutionary hypothesis. J Herpetol 1994, 28:112-114.
  • [13]Brodie ED III: Differential avoidance of coral snake banded patterns by free-ranging avian predators in Costa Rica. Evolution 1993, 47:227-235.
  • [14]Jackson JF, Ingram W III, Campbell HW: The dorsal pigmentation pattern of snakes as an antipredator strategy: a multivariate approach. Am Nat 1976, 110:1029-1053.
  • [15]Allen WL, Baddeley R, Scott-Samuel NE, Cuthill IC: The evolution and function of pattern diversity in snakes. Behav Ecol 2013, 24:1237-1250.
  • [16]How MJ, Zanker JM: Motion camouflage induced by zebra stripes. Zool Jena Ger 2014, 117:163-170.
  • [17]Stevens M, Yule DH, Ruxton GD: Dazzle coloration and prey movement. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2008, 275:2639-2643.
  • [18]Scott-Samuel NE, Baddeley R, Palmer CE, Cuthill IC: Dazzle camouflage affects speed perception. PloS One 2011, 6:e20233.
  • [19]Von Helversen B, Schooler LJ, Czienskowski U: Are stripes beneficial? Dazzle camouflage influences perceived speed and hit rates. PloS One 2013, 8:e61173.
  • [20]Zylinski S, Osorio D, Shohet AJ: Cuttlefish camouflage: context-dependent body pattern use during motion. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 2009, 276:3963-3969.
  • [21]Stevens M, Searle WT, Seymour JE, Marshall KL, Ruxton GD: Motion dazzle and camouflage as distinct anti-predator defenses. BMC Biol 2011, 9:81. BioMed Central Full Text
  • [22]Adelson EH, Movshon JA: Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns. Nature 1982, 300:523-525.
  • [23]Castet E, Lorenceau J, Shiffrar M, Bonnet C: Perceived speed of moving lines depends on orientation, length, speed and luminance. Vision Res 1993, 33:1921-1936.
  • [24]Lorenceau J, Shiffrar M: The influence of terminators on motion integration across space. Vision Res 1992, 32:263-273.
  • [25]Lorenceau J, Shiffrar M, Wells N, Castet E: Different motion sensitive units are involved in recovering the direction of moving lines. Vision Res 1993, 33:1207-1217.
  • [26]Nakayama K, Silverman GH: The aperture problem–I. Perception of nonrigidity and motion direction in translating sinusoidal lines. Vision Res 1988, 28:739-746.
  • [27]Nakayama K, Silverman GH: The aperture problem–II. Spatial integration of velocity information along contours. Vision Res 1988, 28:747-753.
  • [28]Shimojo S, Silverman GH, Nakayama K: Occlusion and the solution to the aperture problem for motion. Vision Res 1989, 29:619-626.
  • [29]Hubel DH, Wiesel TN: Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. J Physiol 1968, 195:215-243.
  • [30]Marr D, Hildreth E: Theory of edge detection. Proc R Soc Lond - Biol Sci 1980, 207:187-217.
  • [31]Hildreth EC: The computation of the velocity field. Proc R Soc Lond - Biol Sci 1984, 221:189-220.
  • [32]Marr D, Ullman S: Directional selectivity and its use in early visual processing. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B Contain Pap Biol Character R Soc G B 1981, 211:151-180.
  • [33]Troscianko J, Lown AE, Hughes AE, Stevens M: Defeating crypsis: detection and learning of camouflage strategies. PLoS One 2013, 8:e73733.
  • [34]Santer RD: Motion dazzle: a locust’s eye view. Biol Lett 2013, 9:20130811-20130811.
  • [35]Kellogg WN: The time of judgment in psychometric measures. Am J Psychol 1931, 43:65-86.
  • [36]Henmon V a C: The relation of the time of a judgment to its accuracy. Psychol Rev 1911, 18:186-201.
  • [37]Audley RJ: A stochastic model for individual choice behavior. Psychol Rev 1960, 67:1-15.
  • [38]Crawley MJ: Statistics: An Introduction Using R. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., Chichester; 2005.
  • [39]Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Walker NJ, Saveliev AA, Smith GM: Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R. Springer, New York; 2009.
  • [40]Ihaka R, Gentleman R: R: A Language for data analysis and graphics. J Comput Graph Stat 1996, 5:299.
  • [41]Ruxton GD, Beauchamp G: Time for some a priori thinking about post hoc testing. Behav Ecol 2008, 19:690-693.
  • [42]Pough FH: Multiple cryptic effects of crossbanded and ringed patterns of snakes. Copeia 1976, 1976:834-836.
  • [43]Derrington AM, Allen HA, Delicato LS: Visual mechanisms of motion analysis and motion perception. Annu Rev Psychol 2004, 55:181-205.
  • [44]Allen HA, Derrington AM: Slow discrimination of contrast-defined expansion patterns. Vision Res 2000, 40:735-744.
  • [45]Ashida H, Seiffert AE, Osaka N: Inefficient visual search for second-order motion. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis 2001, 18:2255-2266.
  • [46]Humphries DA, Driver PM: Erratic display as a device against predators. Science 1967, 156:1767-1768.
  • [47]Tosh CR, Jackson AL, Ruxton GD: The confusion effect in predatory neural networks. Am Nat 2006, 167:E52-E65.
  • [48]Jeschke JM, Tollrian R: Prey swarming: which predators become confused and why? Anim Behav 2007, 74:387-393.
  • [49]Ruxton GD: The possible fitness benefits of striped coat coloration for zebra. Mammal Rev 2002, 32:237-244.
  • [50]Gibson G: Do tsetse-flies see zebras -a field-study of the visual response of tsetse to striped targets. Physiol Entomol 1992, 17:141-147.
  • [51]Caro T, Izzo A, Reiner RC Jr, Walker H, Stankowich T: The function of zebra stripes. Nat Commun 2014, 5:3535.
  • [52]Valkonen J, Niskanen M, Björklund M, Mappes J: Disruption or aposematism? Significance of dorsal zigzag pattern of European vipers. Evol Ecol 2011, 25:1047-1063.
  文献评价指标  
  下载次数:25次 浏览次数:10次