Flavour | |
Assimilation of healthy and indulgent impressions from labelling influences fullness but not intake or sensory experience | |
Martin R. Yeomans1  Peter Hovard1  | |
[1] School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9QH, East Sussex, UK | |
关键词: Health halos; Sensory properties; Expected satiety; Labelling; Satiation; | |
Others : 1228879 DOI : 10.1186/s13411-015-0038-9 |
|
received in 2015-07-10, accepted in 2015-09-24, 发布年份 2015 | |
【 摘 要 】
Background
Recent evidence suggests that products believed to be healthy may be over-consumed relative to believed indulgent or highly caloric products. The extent to which these effects relate to expectations from labelling, oral experience or assimilation of expectations is unclear. Over two experiments, we tested the hypotheses that healthy and indulgent information could be assimilated by oral experience of beverages and influence sensory evaluation, expected satiety, satiation and subsequent appetite. Additionally, we explored how expectation-experience congruency influenced these factors.
Results
Results supported some assimilation of healthiness and indulgent ratings—study 1 showed that indulgent ratings enhanced by the indulgent label persisted post-tasting, and this resulted in increased fullness ratings. In study 2, congruency of healthy labels and oral experience promoted enhanced healthiness ratings. These healthiness and indulgent beliefs did not influence sensory analysis or intake—these were dictated by the products themselves. Healthy labels, but not experience, were associated with decreased expected satiety.
Conclusions
Overall labels generated expectations, and some assimilation where there were congruencies between expectation and experience, but oral experience tended to override initial expectations to determine ultimate sensory evaluations and intake. Familiarity with the sensory properties of the test beverages may have resulted in the use of prior knowledge, rather than the label information, to guide evaluations and behaviour.
【 授权许可】
2015 Hovard and Yeomans.
【 预 览 】
Files | Size | Format | View |
---|---|---|---|
20151020000850305.pdf | 1361KB | download | |
Fig. 6. | 21KB | Image | download |
Fig. 5. | 16KB | Image | download |
Fig. 4. | 22KB | Image | download |
Fig. 3. | 20KB | Image | download |
Fig. 2. | 23KB | Image | download |
Fig. 1. | 16KB | Image | download |
【 图 表 】
Fig. 1.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 3.
Fig. 4.
Fig. 5.
Fig. 6.
【 参考文献 】
- [1]Mozaffarian D, Benjamin EJ, Go AS, Arnett DK, Blaha MJ, Cushman M, Disease H, Statistics S et al.. Update: a report from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2015; 131:e29.
- [2]Grabenhorst F, Schulte FP, Maderwald S, Brand M. Food labels promote healthy choices by a decision bias in the amygdala. Neuroimage. 2013; 74:152-63.
- [3]Shankar MU, Levitan C, Prescott J, Spence C. The influence of color and label information on flavor perception. Chemosens Percept. 2009; 2:53-8.
- [4]Yeomans MR, Chambers L, Blumenthal H, Blake A. The role of expectancy in sensory and hedonic evaluation: the case of smoked salmon ice-cream. Food Qual Prefer. 2008; 19:565-73.
- [5]McCrickerd K, Chambers L, Yeomans MR. Does modifying the thick texture and creamy flavour of a drink change portion size selection and intake? Appetite. 2014; 73:114-20.
- [6]Fay SH, Ferriday D, Hinton EC, Shakeshaft NG, Rogers PJ, Brunstrom JM. What determines real-world meal size? Evidence for pre-meal planning. Appetite. 2011; 56:284-9.
- [7]Chambers L, Ells H, Yeomans MR. Can the satiating power of a high energy beverage be improved by manipulating sensory characteristics and label information? Food Qual Prefer. 2013; 28:271-8.
- [8]McCrickerd K, Chambers L, Yeomans MR. Fluid or fuel? The context of consuming a beverage is important for satiety. PLoS One. 2014;9.
- [9]Yeomans MR, Chambers L. Satiety-relevant sensory qualities enhance the satiating effects of mixed carbohydrate-protein preloads. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011; 94:1410-7.
- [10]Crum AJ, Corbin WR, Brownell KD, Salovey P. Mind over milkshakes: mindsets, not just nutrients, determine ghrelin response. Health Psychol. 2011; 30:424-9.
- [11]Yeomans MR, McCrickerd K, Brunstrom JM, Chambers L. Effects of repeated consumption on sensory-enhanced satiety. Br J Nutr. 2014; 111:1137-44.
- [12]Wooley SC. Physiologic versus cognitive factors in short term food regulation in the obese and nonobese. Psychosom Med. 1972; 34:62-8.
- [13]Chandon P, Wansink B. The biasing health halos of fast-food restaurant health claims: lower calorie estimates and higher side-dish consumption intentions. J Consum Res. 2007; 34:301-14.
- [14]Provencher V, Polivy J, Herman CP. Perceived healthiness of food. If it’s healthy, you can eat more! Appetite. 2009; 52:340-4.
- [15]Wansink B, Chandon P. Can “low-fat” nutrition labels lead to obesity? J Mark Res. 2006; 43:605-17.
- [16]Wansink B, Van Ittersum K, Painter JE. How diet and health labels influence taste and satiation. J Food Sci. 2004; 69:340-6.
- [17]Nisbett RE, Wilson TD. The halo effect: evidence for unconscious alteration of judgments. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1977; 35:250-6.
- [18]Carels R, Harper J, Konrad K. Qualitative perceptions and caloric estimations of healthy and unhealthy foods by behavioral weight loss participants. Appetite. 2006; 46:199-206.
- [19]Roe B, Levy AS, Derby BM. The impact of health claims on consumer search and product evaluation outcomes: results from FDA experimental data. J Public Policy Mark. 1999; 18:89-105.
- [20]Oakes ME, Slotterback CS. Judgements of food healthfulness: food name stereotypes in adults over age 25. Appetite. 2001; 37:1-8.
- [21]Kuenzel J, Zandstra EH, El Deredy W, Blanchette I, Thomas A. Expecting yoghurt drinks to taste sweet or pleasant increases liking. Appetite. 2011; 56:122-7.
- [22]Siegrist M, Cousin ME. Expectations influence sensory experience in a wine tasting. Appetite. 2009; 52:762-5.
- [23]Wansink B, Payne CR, North J. Fine as North Dakota wine: sensory expectations and the intake of companion foods. Physiol Behav. 2007; 90:712-6.
- [24]Wansink B, van Ittersum K, Painter JE. How descriptive food names bias sensory perceptions in restaurants. Food Qual Prefer. 2005; 16:393-400.
- [25]Brunstrom JM, Shakeshaft NG, Scott-Samuel NE. Measuring “expected satiety” in a range of common foods using a method of constant stimuli. Appetite. 2008; 51:604-14.
- [26]Brunstrom JM, Shakeshaft NSG. Measuring affective (liking) and non-affective (expected satiety) determinants of portion size and food reward. Appetite. 2009; 52:108-14.
- [27]Brunstrom JM, Brown S, Hinton EC, Rogers PJ, Fay SH. “Expected satiety” changes hunger and fullness in the inter-meal interval. Appetite. 2011; 56:310-5.
- [28]Brunstrom JM, Shakeshaft NG, Alexander E. Familiarity changes expectations about fullness. Appetite. 2010; 54:587-90.
- [29]Hardman CA, Mccrickerd K, Brunstrom JM. Children’s familiarity with snack foods changes expectations. Am J Clin Nutr. 2011; 94:1196-201.
- [30]Mccrickerd K, Chambers L, Brunstrom JM, Yeomans MR. Subtle changes in the flavour and texture of a drink enhance expectations of satiety. Flavour. 2012; 1:1-11. BioMed Central Full Text
- [31]Bertenshaw EJ, Lluch A, Yeomans MR. Perceived thickness and creaminess modulates the short-term satiating effects of high-protein drinks. Br J Nutr. 2013; 110:578-86.
- [32]Klaaren KJ, Hodges SD, Wilson TD. The role of affective expectations in subjective experience and decision-making. Soc Cogn. 1994; 12:77-101.
- [33]Wilson TD, Lisle DJ, Kraft D, Wetzel CG. Preferences as expectation-driven inferences: effects of affective expectations on affective experience. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989; 56:519-30.
- [34]Zellner DA, Strickhouser D, Tornow CE. Disconfirmed hedonic expectations produce perceptual contrast, not assimilation. Am J Psychol. 2004; 117:363-87.
- [35]Stunkard AJ, Messick S. The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res. 1985; 29:71-83.
- [36]Cassady BA, Considine RV, Mattes RD. Beverage consumption, appetite, and energy intake: what did you expect? Am J Clin Nutr. 2012; 95:587-93.
- [37]Peracchio LA, Tybout AM. The moderating role of prior knowledge in schema-based product evaluation. J Consum Res. 1996; 23:177-92.
- [38]Brunstrom JM, Collingwood J, Rogers PJ. Perceived volume, expected satiation, and the energy content of self-selected meals. Appetite. 2010; 55:25-9.
- [39]De Castro JM. Palatability and intake relationships in free-living humans: the influence of heredity. Nutr Res. 2001; 21:935-45.
- [40]Caputo FA, Mattes RD. Human dietary responses to perceived manipulation of fat content in a midday meal. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 1993; 17:237-40.
- [41]Yeomans MR, Lartamo S, Procter EL, Lee MD, Gray RW. The actual, but not labelled, fat content of a soup preload alters short-term appetite in healthy men. Physiol Behav. 2001; 73:533-40.
- [42]Shankar MU, Levitan C, Spence C. Grape expectations: the role of cognitive influences in color-flavor interactions. Consc Cog. 2010; 19:380-90.
- [43]Ogden J, Wardle J. Cognitive restraint and sensitivity to cues for hunger and satiety. Physiol Behav. 1990; 47:477-481.
- [44]Hill AJ, Magson LD, Blundell JE. Hunger and palatability: tracking ratings of subjective experience before, during and after the consumption of preferred and less preferred food. Appetite. 1984; 5:361-71.
- [45]Yeomans MR. Rating changes over the course of meals: what do they tell us about motivation to eat? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2000; 24:249-59.