期刊论文详细信息
BMC Public Health
Maternal education, anthropometric markers of malnutrition and cognitive function (ELSA-Brasil)
Sandhi Maria Barreto3  Valéria Maria Azeredo Passos3  Dora Chor1  Luana Giatti2  Larissa Fortunato Araújo3 
[1] National School of Public Health, Fundação Oswaldo Cruz, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil;School of Nutrition, Universidade Federal de Ouro Preto, Minas Gerais, Brazil;Research Group on Epidemiology of Chronic Diseases (GERMINAL), Faculty of Medicine, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Minas Gerais, Brazil
关键词: Cognitive function;    Life course;    Nutritional markers;    Maternal education;   
Others  :  866325
DOI  :  10.1186/1471-2458-14-673
 received in 2013-09-08, accepted in 2014-06-25,  发布年份 2014
PDF
【 摘 要 】

Background

The early exposure to poor social and nutritional conditions may influence cognitive function during adult age. However, the relative impact of these factors has not yet been established and they can vary during the course of life.

Methods

Analysis of data from 12,997 participants (35-64 years) of the baseline exams (2008-2010) of the Longitudinal Study of Adult Health (ELSA-Brasil), a cohort of Brazilian civil servants. Four cognitive tests were applied: learning, recall and word recognition; semantic and phonemic verbal fluency; trail-making test version B. The markers of early nutritional and social conditions were maternal educational level, birth weight, and length of trunk and leg. The presence of independent association between every early marker and the poor performance in each cognitive test was investigated by multiple logistic regression, after mutual adjustment and considering the effects of gender, age and participant’s schooling level. The cut off for poor performance was the worst age-specific percentile of the final score distribution for each test.

Results

After full adjustments, lower maternal education increased the chances of poor performance in all cognitive tests, with a dose-response gradient; low birth-weight was related to poor performance in the trail-making test B (OR = 1.63, 95% IC = 1.29-2.06); and greater trunk length decreased the chances of poor performance in the semantic and phonemic verbal fluency (OR = 0.96, 95% IC = 0.94-0.97) and in the trail-making test B (OR = 0.94, 95% IC = 0.92-0.95). Leg length was not associated with any of the tests examined. The associations found were not modified by the educational attainment of the participants.

Conclusions

Early exposure to adverse social and nutritional conditions appear detrimental to semantic memory, learning, concentration, executive control and language among adults, independent of adulthood educational achievement.

【 授权许可】

   
2014 Araújo et al.; licensee BioMed Central Ltd.

【 预 览 】
附件列表
Files Size Format View
20140727062050205.pdf 235KB PDF download
【 参考文献 】
  • [1]Sliwinski MJ, Hofer SM, Hall C, Buschke H, Lipton RB: Modelling memory decline in older adults: the importance of preclinical dementia, attrition, and chronological age. Psychol Aging 2003, 18:658-671.
  • [2]Steinerman JR, Hall CB, Sliwinski MJ, Lipton RB: Modelling cognitive trajectories within longitudinal studies: a focus on older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2010, 58(2):313-318.
  • [3]Hackman DA, Farah MJ, Meaney MJ: Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010, 11(9):651-659.
  • [4]Singh-Manoux A, Richards M, Marmot M: Socioeconomic position across the life course: how does it relate to cognitive function in mid-life? Ann Epidemiol 2005, 15:572-578.
  • [5]Hazzouri AZ, Haan MN, Galea S, Aiello AE: Life-Course Exposure to Early Socioeconomic Environment, Education in Relation to Late-Life Cognitive Function Among Older Mexicans and Mexican Americans. J Aging Health 2011, 23(7):1027-1049.
  • [6]Brunner EJ: Social and biological determinants of cognitive aging. Neurobiol Aging 2005, 26:17-20.
  • [7]Kaplan GA, Turrell G, Lynch JW, Everson SA, Helkala EL, Salonen JT: Childhood socioeconomic position and cognitive function in adulthood. Int J Epidemiol 2001, 30:256-263.
  • [8]Melrose RJ, Brewster P, Marquine MJ, Mackay Brandt A, Reed BT, Farias S, Mungas D: Early Life Development in a Multiethnic Sample and the Relation to Late Life Cognition. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2013. doi:10.1093/geronb/gbt126
  • [9]Victora CG, Adair L, Fall C: Maternal and child under nutrition for adult health and human capital. Lancet 2008, 371(9609):302-02.
  • [10]Monk C, Georgieffc MK, Osterholm EA: Maternal prenatal distress and poor nutrition – mutually influencing risk factors affecting infant neurocognitive development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2013, 54(2):115-130.
  • [11]Torche CF, Echevarrıa G: The effect of birth weight on childhood cognitive development in a middle-income. Int J Epidemiol 2011, 40:1008-1018.
  • [12]Mak Z, Kim JM, Stewart R: Leg length, cognitive impairment and cognitive decline in an African-Caribbean population. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2006, 21:266-272.
  • [13]Abbott R, White L, Ross G, Petrovich H, Masaki K, Snowdon D, Curb J: Height as a marker of childhood development and late-life cognitive function: the Honolulu-Asia aging study. Pediatrics 1998, 102:602-609.
  • [14]Schnaider Beeri M, Davidson M, Silverman J, Noy S, Schmeidler J, Goldbourt U: Relationship between body height and dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatr 2005, 13:116-123.
  • [15]Case A, Paxson C: Height, health and cognitive function at older ages. Am Econ Rev Pap Proc 2008, 98:463-467.
  • [16]Glymour MM, Manly JJ: Life course Social Conditions and Racial and Ethnic Patterns of Cognitive Aging. Neuropsychol Rev 2008, 18:223-254.
  • [17]Farah MJ, Betancourt L, Shera DM, Savage JH, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Malmud EK, Hurt H: Environmental stimulation, parental nurturance and cognitive development in humans. Dev Sci 2008, 11(5):793-801.
  • [18]Wilson RS, Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Barnes LL, Mendes De Leon CF, Evans DA: Educational attainment and cognitive decline in old age. Neurology 2009, 72(3):460-465.
  • [19]Bloom DE, Canning D, Fink G: Implications of population ageing for economic growth. Oxf Rev Econ Policy 2010, 26(4):583-612.
  • [20]Hackman DA, Farah MJ: Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends Cogn Sci 2009, 13(2):65-73.
  • [21]Lupu DS, Tint D, Niculescu MD: Perinatal Epigenetic Determinants of Cognitive and Metabolic Disorders. Ageing Dis 2012, 3(5):444-453.
  • [22]Ferri CP, Prince M, Brayne C, Brodaty H, Fratiglioni L, Ganguli M, Hall K, Hasegawa K, Hendrie H, Huang Y, Jorm A, Mathers C, Menezes PR, Rimmer E, Scazufca M: Global prevalence of dementia: a Delphi consensus study. Lancet 2005, 366:2112-2117.
  • [23]Maurer J: Height, education and later-life cognition in Latin America and the Caribbean. Econ Hum Biol 2010, 8:168-176.
  • [24]McEniry M: Early-life conditions and older adult health in low- and middle income countries: a review. J Dev Orig Health Dis 2013, 4(1):10-29.
  • [25]Aquino EML, Barreto SM, Bensenor IM, Carvalho MS, Chor D, Duncan BB, Lotufo PA, Mill JG, Molina MDC, Mota ELA, Passos VMAP, Schmidt MI, Szklo M: Brazilian Longitudinal Study of Adult Health (ELSA-Brasil): Objectives and Design. Am J Epidemiol 2012. doi:10.1093/aje/kwr294
  • [26]Schmidt MI, Duncan BB, Mill JG, Lotufo PA, Chor D, Barreto SM, Aquino EML, Passos VMA, Matos SMM, Molina MDCB, Carvalho MS, Bensenor IM: Cohort Profile: Longitudinal Study of Adult Health (ELSA-Brasil). Int J Epidemiol 2014. doi:10.1093/ije/dyu027
  • [27]Bertolucci PHF, Okamoto IH, Toniolo-Neto J, Ramos LR, Brucki SMD: Desempenho da população brasileira na bateria neuropsicológica do Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Revista Psiquiatria Clínica 1998, 25(2):80-83.
  • [28]Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW: Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th edition. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2004.
  • [29]Batista JA, Giatti L, Barreto SM, Passos VMA: Confiabilidade dos testes da bateria de função cognitiva do Estudo Longitudinal de Saúde do Adulto-Brasil. Article Submitted 2013, 1:1-12.
  • [30]Wadsworth ME, Hardy RJ, Paul AA, Marshall SF, Cole TJ: Leg and trunk length at 43 years in relation to childhood health, diet and family circumstances; evidence from the 1946 national birth cohort. Int J Epidemiol 2002, 31:383-390.
  • [31]Rubim BD: Multiple imputation for non-response in surveys. New York: Wiley; 1987.
  • [32]Buren SV: Groothuis-Oudshoorn K: mice: Multivariate Imputation by Chained Equations in R. J Stat Softw 2011, 45(3):1-67.
  • [33]Kuh D, Ben-Shlomo Y: A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1997.
  • [34]Zhao JH, Brunner EJ, Kumari M, Singh-Manoux A, Hawe E, Talmud PJ, Marmot MG, Humphries SE: APOE polymorphism, socioeconomic status and cognitive function in midlife: the Whitehall II longitudinal study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatry Epidemiol 2005, 40:557-563.
  • [35]Anstey KJ, Luszcz MA, Giles LC, Andrews GR: Demographic, health, cognitive, and sensory variables as predictors of mortality in very old adults. Psychol Aging 2001, 16:3-11.
  • [36]Turrell G, Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Everson SA, Helkala E-L, Kauhanen J, Salonen JT: Socioeconomic position across the life course and cognitive function in late middle age. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2002, 57:43-51.
  • [37]Rogers MAM, Plassman BL, Kabeto M, Fisher GG, McArdle JJ, Llewllyn DJ, Langa KM: Parental Education and Late-life Dementia in the United States. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2009, 22(1):71-80.
  • [38]Shmueli A, Cullen MR: Birth Weight, Maternal Age, and Education: New Observations from Connecticut and Virginia. Yale J Biol Med 1999, 72:245-258.
  • [39]Victora CG, Huttly SR, Barros FC, Lombardi C, Vaughan P: Maternal education in relation to early and late child health outcomes: findings from a Brazilian cohort study. Soc Sci Med 1992, 34(8):899-905.
  • [40]Dubow EF, Boxer P, Huesmann LR: Long-term Effects of Parents’ Education on Children’s Educational and Occupational Success: Mediation by Family Interactions, Child Aggression, and Teenage Aspirations. Merrill Palmer Q (Wayne State Univ Press) 2009, 55(3):224-249.
  • [41]Jefferson AL, Gibbons LE, Rentz DM, Carvalho JO, Manly J, Bennett DA, Jones RN: A Life Course Model of Cognitive Activities, Socioeconomic Status, Education, Reading Ability, and Cognition. J Am Geriatr Soc 2011, 59(8):1403-1124.
  • [42]Byford M, Kuh D, Richards M: Parenting practices and intergenerational associations in cognitive ability. Int J Epidemiol 2011, 41(1):263-272.
  • [43]Clair-Thompson HL, Gathercole SE: Executive functions and achievements in school: shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol (Colch) 2006, 59(4):745-749.
  • [44]Raikkonen K, Kajantie E, Pesonen A-K, Heinonen K, Alastalo H, Leskinen JT, Nyman K, Henriksson M, Lahti J, Lahti M, Pyhala R, Tovinen S, Osmond C, Barker DJP, Eriksson JG: Early life Origins Cognitive Decline: Findings in Elderly Men in the Helsinki Birth Cohort Study. PLoS One 2013, 8(1):e54707. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0054707
  • [45]Bogin B, Varela-Silva MI: Leg Length, Body Proportion, and Health: A Review with a Note on Beauty. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2010, 7(3):1047-1075.
  • [46]Gigante DP, Nazmi A, Lima RC, Barros FC, Victora CG: Epidemiology of early and late growth in height, leg and trunk length: findings from a birth cohort of Brazilian males. Eur J Clin Nutr 2009, 63(3):375-381.
  • [47]Quan SA, Jeong JY, Kim DH: The Relationship between Height and Cognitive Function among Community-dwelling Elderly: Hallym Aging Study. Epidemiol Health 2013. doi:10.4178/epih/e2013002
  • [48]Laitala VS, Hjelmborg J, Koskenvuo M, Räihä I, Rinne JO, Christensen K, Kaprio J, Silventoinen K: Shorter adult stature increases the impact of risk factors for cognitive impairment: a comparison of two Nordic twin cohorts. Twin Res Hum Genet 2011, 14(6):544-552.
  • [49]Heys M, Schooling M, Jiang C, Adad P, Cheng KK, Lam TH, Leung GM: Childhood growth and adulthood cognition in a rapidly developing population. Epidemiology 2009, 20:91-99.
  • [50]Li L, Dangour AD, Power C: Early life influences on adult leg and trunk length in the 1958 British birth cohort. Am J Hum Biol 2007, 19(6):836-843.
  • [51]Barros RP, Henriques R, Mendonça R: Pelo fim das décadas perdidas: Educação e desenvolvimento sustentado no Brasil. Destrito Federal, DF: Ministério do Planejamento, Orçamento e Gestão; 2002. [Texto para discussão 857]
  • [52]Monteiro CA, Benicio MH, Iunes R, Gouveia NC, Taddei JA, Cardoso MA: Nutritional status of Brazilian children: trends from 1975 to 1989. Bull World Health Organ 1992, 70(5):657-666.
  • [53]Santos IS, Griep RH, Alves MG, Goulart AC, Lotufo PA, Barreto SM, Chor D, Benseñor IM: Job stress is associated with migraine in current workers: The Brazilian Longitudinal Study of Adult Health (ELSA-Brasil). Eur J Pain 2014. doi:10.1002/j.1532-2149.2014.489.x
  • [54]Mueller NT, Duncan BB, Barreto SM, Chor D, Bessel M, Aquino EM, Pereira MA, Schmidt M: Earlier age at menarche is associated with higher diabetes risk and cardiometabolic disease risk factors in Brazilian adults: Brazilian Longitudinal Study of Adult Health (ELSA-Brasil). Cardiovasc Diabetol 2014, 16:13-22.
  • [55]Rothman KJ, Gallacher JEJ, Hatch E: Why representativeness should be avoided. Int J Epidemiol 2013, 42:1012-1014.
  • [56]Richiardi L, Pizzi C, Pearce N: Commentary: Representativeness is usually not necessary and often should be avoided. Int J Epidemiol 2013, 42(4):1018-1022.
  • [57]Donders AR, van der Heijden GJ, Stijnen T, Moons KG: Review: a gentle introduction to imputation of missing values. J Clin Epidemiol 2006, 59(10):1087-1091.
  文献评价指标  
  下载次数:0次 浏览次数:14次