期刊论文详细信息
BMC Evolutionary Biology
Avoiding perceived past resource use of potential competitors affects niche dynamics in a bird community
Blandine Doligez1  Lars Gustafsson4  Janne-Tuomas Seppänen2  Tuomo Jaakkonen5  Sami M Kivelä3  Jukka T Forsman6 
[1] Department of Biometry and Evolutionary Biology, CNRS, University of Lyon, Villeurbenne Cedex, F-69622, France;Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland;Current address: Department of Zoology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, SE-10691, Sweden;Department of Ecology and Genetics/Animal Ecology, EBC, Uppsala Universit, Norbyvägen 18D, Uppsala, SE-752 36, Sweden;Department of Biology, University of Oulu, Oulu, FI-90014, Finland;Department of Biology, Section of Ecology, University of Turku, Turku, FI-20014, Finland
关键词: Ficedula;    Parus;    Cavity nesting birds;    Nest-site selection;    Niche division;    Intra- and interspecific competition;    Resource partitioning;    Social information use;    Species interactions;   
Others  :  1118011
DOI  :  10.1186/s12862-014-0175-2
 received in 2014-04-30, accepted in 2014-07-24,  发布年份 2014
PDF
【 摘 要 】

Background

Social information use is usually considered to lead to ecological convergence among involved con- or heterospecific individuals. However, recent results demonstrate that observers can also actively avoid behaving as those individuals being observed, leading to ecological divergence. This phenomenon has been little explored so far, yet it can have significant impact on resource use, realized niches and species co-existence. In particular, the time-scale and the ecological context over which such shifts can occur are unknown. We examined with a long-term (four years) field experiment whether experimentally manipulated, species-specific, nest-site feature preferences (symbols on nest boxes) are transmitted across breeding seasons and affect future nest-site preferences in a guild of three cavity-nesting birds.

Results

Of the examined species, resident great tits (Parus major) preferred the symbol that had been associated with unoccupied nest boxes in the previous year, i.e., their preference shifted towards niche space previously unused by putative competitors and conspecifics.

Conclusions

Our results show that animals can remember the earlier resource use of conspecifics and other guild members and adjust own decisions accordingly one year after. Our experiment cannot reveal the ultimate mechanism(s) behind the observed behaviour but avoiding costs of intra- or interspecific competition or ectoparasite load in old nests are plausible reasons. Our findings imply that interspecific social information use can affect resource sharing and realized niches in ecological time-scale through active avoidance of observed decisions and behavior of potentially competing species.

【 授权许可】

   
2014 Forsman et al.; BioMed Central Ltd

【 预 览 】
附件列表
Files Size Format View
20150206015709187.pdf 482KB PDF download
Figure 4. 22KB Image download
Figure 3. 116KB Image download
Figure 2. 57KB Image download
Figure 1. 58KB Image download
【 图 表 】

Figure 1.

Figure 2.

Figure 3.

Figure 4.

【 参考文献 】
  • [1]Wcislo WT: Behavioural environments and evolutionary change. Ann Rev Ecol Syst 1989, 20:137-169.
  • [2]Price TD, Qvarnström A, Irwin DE: The role of phenotypic plasticity in driving genetic evolution. Proc R Soc Lond B 2003, 270:1433-1440.
  • [3]Galef BG Jr, Laland KN: Social learning in animals: empirical studies and theoretical models. Bioscience 2005, 55:489-499.
  • [4]Danchin É, Giraldeau LA, Valone TJ, Wagner RH: Public information: from nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 2004, 305:487-491.
  • [5]Dall SRX, Giraldeau L-A, Olsson O, McNamara JM, Stephens DW: Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol Evol 2005, 20:187-193.
  • [6]Seppänen J–T, Forsman JT, Mönkkönen M, Krams I, Salmi T: New behavioural trait adopted or rejected by observing heterospecific tutor fitness. Proc R Soc Lond B 2011, 278:1736-1741.
  • [7]Seppänen J–T, Forsman JT, Mönkkönen M, Thomson RL: Social information use is a process across space, time and ecology, reaching heterospecifics. Ecology 2007, 88:1622-1633.
  • [8]Goodale E, Beauchamp G, Magrath RD, Nieh JC, Ruxton GD: Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. Trends Ecol Evol 2010, 25:354-361.
  • [9]Slaa EJ, Hughes WHO: Local enhancement, local inhibition, eavesdropping, and the parasitism of social insect communication. In Food exploitation by social insects. Edited by Jarau S, Hrncir M. CRC Press, Boca Raton; 2009:147-164.
  • [10]MacArthur R, Levins R: The limiting similarity, convergence, and divergence of coexisting species. Am Nat 1967, 101:377-385.
  • [11]Schoener TW: Resource partitioning in ecological communities. Science 1974, 185:27-39.
  • [12]Alatalo RV, Eriksson D, Gustafsson L, Larsson K: Exploitation competition influences the use of foraging sites by tits: Experimental evidence. Ecology 1987, 68:284-290.
  • [13]Losos JB, Leal M, Glor RE, de Queiroz K, Hertz PE, Schettino LR, Lara AC, Jackman TR, Larson A: Niche lability in the evolution of Caribbean lizard community. Nature 2003, 424:542-545.
  • [14]Cavender-Bares J, Ackerl DD, Baum DA, Bazzaz FA: Phylogenetic overdispersion in Floridian oak communities. Am Nat 2004, 163:823-843.
  • [15]Mönkkönen M, Härdling R, Forsman JT, Tuomi J: Evolution of heterospecific attraction: using other species as cues in habitat selection. Evol Ecol 1999, 13:91-104.
  • [16]Seppänen J–T, Forsman JT: Interspecific social learning: Novel preference can be acquired from a competing species. Curr Biol 2007, 17:1248-1252.
  • [17]Slagsvold T, Wiebe K: Learning the ecological niche. Proc R Soc Lond B 2007, 274:19-23.
  • [18]Forsman JT, Seppänen J–T: Learning what (not) to do: testing rejection and copying of heterospecific behavioural trait. Anim Behav 2011, 81:879-883.
  • [19]Loukola OJ, Seppänen J-T, Krams I, Torvinen S, Forsman JT: Observed tutor fitness may drive niche convergence and divergence in competing species via selective social information use. Am Nat 2013, 182:474-483.
  • [20]Dhondt A: Interspecific competition between great and blue tit. Nature 1977, 268:521-523.
  • [21]Sasvári L, Török J, Tóth L: Density dependent effects between three competitive bird species. Oecologia 1987, 72:127-130.
  • [22]Slagsvold T: Competition between the Great Tit Parus major and the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca in the breeding season. Ornis Scand 1975, 6:179-190.
  • [23]Gustafsson L: Interspecific competition lowers fitness in collared flycatchers Ficedula albicollis: an experimental demonstration. Ecology 1987, 68:291-296.
  • [24]Forsman JT, Hjernquist MB, Taipale Gustafsson L: Competitor density cues for habitat quality facilitating habitat selection and investment decisions. Behav Ecol 2008, 19:539-545.
  • [25]Forsman JT, Seppänen J–T, Mönkkönen M: Positive fitness consequences of interspecific interaction with a potential competitor. Proc R Soc Lond B 2002, 269:1619-1623.
  • [26]Forsman JT, Thomson RL, Seppänen J-T: Mechanisms and fitness effects of interspecific information use between migrant and resident birds. Behav Ecol 2007, 18:888-894.
  • [27]Martin TE: Fitness costs of resource overlap among coexisting bird species. Nature 1996, 380:338-340.
  • [28]Martin TE: Are microhabitat preferences of coexisting species under selection and adaptive? Ecology 1998, 79:656-670.
  • [29]Jaenike J, Holt RD: Genetic-variation for habitat preference - evidence and explanations. Am Nat 1991, 137:S67-S90.
  • [30]Loukola OJ, Seppänen J–T, Forsman JT: Intraspecific social information use in the selection of nest site characteristics. Anim Behav 2012, 83:629-633.
  • [31]Chase JM, Leibold MA: Ecological Niches. Linking Classical and Contemporary Approaches. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago; 2003.
  • [32]Bolnick DI: Intraspecific competition favours niche width expansion in Drosophila melanogaster. Nature 2001, 410:563-466.
  • [33]Svanbäck R, Bolnick DI: Intraspecific competition drives increased resource use diversity within a natural population. Proc R Soc Lond B 2007, 274:839-844.
  • [34]Bolnick DI, Ingram T, Stutz WE, Snowber LK, Lau OL, Paull JS: Ecological release from interspecific competition leads to decoupled changes in population and individual niche width. Proc R Soc Lond B 2010, 277:1789-1797.
  • [35]Lemoine M, Doligez B, Richner H: On the equivalence of host local adaptation and parasite maladaptation: an experimental test. Am Nat 2012, 179:270-281.
  • [36]Pärt T: The importance of local familiarity and search costs for age- and sex-biased philopatry in the collared flycatcher. Anim Behav 1995, 49:1029-1038.
  • [37]Török J, Toth L: Density dependence in reproduction of the collared flycatcher (Ficedula albicollis) at high population levels. J Anim Ecol 1988, 57:251-258.
  • [38]Holt RD: Predation, apparent competition, and the structure of prey communities. Theor Pop Biol 1975, 12:197-229.
  • [39]Loukola OJ, Laaksonen T, Seppänen J-T, Forsman JT: Active hiding of social information from information parasites. BMC Evol Biol 2014, 14:32. BioMed Central Full Text
  • [40]Laland KN: Social learning strategies. Anim Learn Behav 2004, 32:4-14.
  • [41]Doligez B, Danchin E, Clobert J: Public information and breeding habitat selection in a wild bird population. Science 2002, 297:1168-1170.
  • [42]Doligez B, Pärt T, Danchin E, Clobert J, Gustafsson L: Availability and use of public information and conspecific density for settlement decisions in the collared flycatcher. J Anim Ecol 2004, 73:75-87.
  • [43]Parejo D, White J, Clobert J, Dreiss A, Danchin E: Blue tits use fledgling quantity and quality as public information in breeding site choice. Ecology 2007, 88:2373-2382.
  • [44]Kivelä SM, Seppänen J-T, Ovaskainen O, Doligez B, Gustafsson L, Mönkkönen M, Forsman JT: The past and the present in decision-making: the use of con- and heterospecific cues in nest-site selection.Ecology 2014. in press.
  • [45]Doligez B, Pärt T, Danchin E: Prospecting in the collared flycatcher: gathering public information for future breeding habitat selection? Anim Behav 2004, 67:457-466.
  • [46]R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Development Core Team, Vienna, Austria; 2012.
  • [47]Hadfield J: MCMC methods for Multi-response Generalised Linear Mixed Models: The MCMCglmm R Package. J Stat Software 2010, 33:1-22.
  • [48]Plummer M, Best N, Cowles K, Vines K: CODA: Convergence Diagnosis and Output Analysis for MCMC. R News 2006, 6:7-11.
  文献评价指标  
  下载次数:31次 浏览次数:20次