期刊论文详细信息
BMC Evolutionary Biology
Context-specific close-range “hoo” calls in wild gibbons (Hylobates lar)
Klaus Zuberbühler2  Ulrich H Reichard1  Esther Clarke3 
[1] Department of Anthropology and left for Ecology, University of Southern Illinois, Carbondale, USA;Cognitive Science Centre, University of Neuchâtel, Neuchâtel, Switzerland;School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St Andrews, St Andrews KY16 9JP, UK
关键词: Language evolution;    Ape communication;    Hylobatidae;    Primate vocalisations;    Referential communication;   
Others  :  1172033
DOI  :  10.1186/s12862-015-0332-2
 received in 2014-11-18, accepted in 2015-03-09,  发布年份 2015
PDF
【 摘 要 】

Background

Close range calls are produced by many animals during intra-specific interactions, such as during home range defence, playing, begging for food, and directing others. In this study, we investigated the most common close range vocalisation of lar gibbons (Hylobates lar), the ‘hoo’ call. Gibbons and siamangs (family Hylobatidae) are known for their conspicuous and elaborate songs, while quieter, close range vocalisations have received almost no empirical attention, perhaps due to the difficult observation conditions in their natural forest habitats.

Results

We found that ‘hoo’ calls were emitted by both sexes in a variety of contexts, including feeding, separation from group members, encountering predators, interacting with neighbours, or as part of duet songs by the mated pair. Acoustic analyses revealed that ‘hoo’ calls varied in a number of spectral parameters as a function of the different contexts. Males’ and females’ ‘hoo’ calls showed similar variation in these context-specific parameter differences, although there were also consistent sex differences in frequency across contexts.

Conclusions

Our study provides evidence that lar gibbons are able to generate significant, context-dependent acoustic variation within their main social call, which potentially allows recipients to make inferences about the external events experienced by the caller. Communicating about different events by producing subtle acoustic variation within some call types appears to be a general feature of primate communication, which can increase the expressive power of vocal signals within the constraints of limited vocal tract flexibility that is typical for all non-human primates. In this sense, this study is of direct relevance for the on-going debate about the nature and origins of vocally-based referential communication and the evolution of human speech.

【 授权许可】

   
2015 Clarke et al.; licensee BioMed Central.

【 预 览 】
附件列表
Files Size Format View
20150421010403427.pdf 1069KB PDF download
Figure 4. 12KB Image download
Figure 3. 31KB Image download
Figure 2. 32KB Image download
Figure 1. 63KB Image download
【 图 表 】

Figure 1.

Figure 2.

Figure 3.

Figure 4.

【 参考文献 】
  • [1]Hurford JR: The origins of meaning. Oxford University Press 2007.
  • [2]Macedonia JM, Evans CS: Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Ethology 1993, 93:177-197.
  • [3]Struhsaker TT: Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (cercopithecus aethiops). In Social communication among primates. Edited by Altmann SA. University of Chicago Press, Chicago; 1967:281-324.
  • [4]Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P: Vervet monkey alarm calls - semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim Behav 1980, 28(NOV):1070-1094.
  • [5]Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM: How monkeys see the world. University of Chicago Press, Chicago; 1990.
  • [6]Gouzoules S, Gouzoules H, Marler P: Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) screams: representational signalling in the recruitment of agonistic aid. Anim Behav 1984, 32(1):182-193.
  • [7]Zuberbühler K: Referential labelling in diana monkeys. Anim Behav 2000, 59:917-927.
  • [8]Di Bitetti MS: Food-associated calls of tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus) are functionally referential signals. Behaviour 2003, 140:565-592.
  • [9]Kirchhof J, Hammerschmidt K: Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax) - evidence from playback experiments. Ethology 2006, 112(4):346-354.
  • [10]Crockford C, Boesch C: Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: analysis of barks. Anim Behav 2003, 66:115-125.
  • [11]Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K: Functionally referential communication in a chimpanzee. Curr Biol 2005, 15(19):1779-1784.
  • [12]Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K: Agonistic screams in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) vary as a function of social role. J Comp Psychol 2005, 119(1):67-77.
  • [13]Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K: Food-associated calls in chimpanzees: responses to food types or food preferences? Anim Behav 2006, 72(5):989-999.
  • [14]Notman H, Rendall D: Contextual variation in chimpanzee pant hoots and its implications for referential communication. Anim Behav 2005, 70:177-190.
  • [15]Faragó T, Pongrácz P, Range F, Virányi Z, Miklósi A: 'The bone is mine': affective and referrential aspects of dog growls. Anim Behav 2010, 79:917-925.
  • [16]Janik VM: Cognitive skills in bottlenose dolphic communication. Trends Cogn Sci 2013, 17(4):157-159.
  • [17]Wilson DR, Evans CS: Fowl communicate the size, speed and proximity of avian stimuli through graded structure in referential alarm calls. Anim Behav 2012, 83:535-544.
  • [18]Griesser M: Referential calls signal predator behavior in a group-living bird species. Current biology :CB 2008, 18:69-73.
  • [19]Templeton CN, Greene E, Davis K: Allometry of alarm calls: Black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 2005, 308(5730):1934-1937.
  • [20]Zuberbühler K: A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Anim Behav 2002, 63(2):293-299.
  • [21]Zuberbühler K: Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proc R Soc London, Ser B 2000, 267(1444):713-718.
  • [22]Rainey HJ, Zuberbühler K, Slater PJB: Hornbills can distinguish between primate alarm calls. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci 2004, 271:755-759.
  • [23]Clarke E: The vocalisations and anti-predatory behaviourof wild white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) in Khao Yai National Park. University of St Andrews, Thailand. PhD; 2010.
  • [24]Wheeler BC, Fischer J: Functionally referential signals: a promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evolutionary Anthropology 2012, 21:195-205.
  • [25]Townsend SW, Manser MB: Functionally referential communication in mammals: the past, present and the future. Ethology 2013, 119:1-11.
  • [26]Fitch WT: The evolution of language: a comparative review. Biol Philos 2005, 20(2–3):193-230.
  • [27]Maynard-Smith J: The evolution of alarm calls. Am Nat 1965, 99:59-63.
  • [28]The evolution of primate societies. Univerity of Chicago Press, Chicago; 2012.
  • [29]Mccomb K, Semple S: Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biol Lett 2005, 1(4):381-385.
  • [30]Slocombe KE, Zuberbühler K: Vocal communication in chimpanzees. In The mind of the chimpanzee. Edited by Lonsdorf EV, Ross SR, Matsuzawa T. University of Chicago Press, Chicago; 2010:192-207.
  • [31]Zuberbühler K, Lemasson A: Primate communication: Meaning from strings of calls. In Language and recursion. Edited by Lowenthal F, Lefebvre L. Springer, New York; 2014:115-125.
  • [32]Candiotti A, Zuberbühler K, Lemasson A: Convergence and divergence in diana monkey vocalizations. Biol Lett 2012, 8:382-385.
  • [33]Clay Z, Zuberbühler K: Food-associated calling sequences on bonobos. Anim Behav 2009, 77:1387-1396.
  • [34]Clay Z, Zuberbühler K: Bonobos extract meaning from call sequences. PLoS One 2011, 6(4):e18786.
  • [35]Mitani JC: Gibbon song duets and intergroup spacing. Behaviour 1985, 82:59-96.
  • [36]Raemaekers PM, Raemaekers JJ: Long-range vocal interactions between groups of gibbons (Hylobates lar). Behaviour 1985, 95:26-44.
  • [37]Clarke E, Reichard UH, Zuberbühler K: The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon song. PLoS One 2006, 1(1):e73.
  • [38]Koda H, Nishimura T, Tokuda IT, Oyakawa C, Nihonmatsu T, Masataka N: Soprano singing in gibbons. Am J Phys Anthropol 2012, 149(3):347-355.
  • [39]Koda H, Lemasson A, Oyakawa C, Rizaldi , Pamungkas J, Masataka N: Possible role of mother-daughter vocal interactions on the development of species-specific song in gibbons. PLoS One 2013, 8(8):e71432.
  • [40]Carpenter CR: A field study in siam of the behavior and social relations of the gibbon (Hylobates lar). Comparative Psychology Monographs 1940, 16(5):1-212.
  • [41]Ellefson JO: A natural history of white-handed gibbons in the malayan peninsula. Gibbon and siamang 1974, 3:2-134.
  • [42]Raemaekers JJ, Raemaekers PM, Haimoff EH: Loud calls of the gibbon (Hylobates lar) - repertoire, organization and context. Behaviour 1984, 91:146-189.
  • [43]Leigh SR, Shea BT: Ontogeny and the evolution of adult body size dimorphism in apes. Am J Primatol 1995, 36(1):37-60.
  • [44]Reichard UH, Ganpanakngan M, Barelli C: White-handed gibbons of Khao Yai: Social flexibility, complex reproductive strategies, and a slow life history. 2012.
  • [45]Haimoff EH: The organization of song in the agile gibbon (Hylobates agilis). Folia Primatol 1984, 42(1):42-61.
  • [46]Oyakawa C, Koda H, Sugiura H: Acoustic features contributing to the individuality of wild agile gibbon (Hylobates agilis agilis) songs. Am J Primatol 2007, 69(7):777-790.
  • [47]Mitani JC: Gibbon song duets and intergroup spacing. Behaviour 1985, 92:59-96.
  • [48]Clarke E, Reichard UH, Zuberbühler K: The anti-predator behaviour of wild white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 2012, 66:85-96.
  • [49]Jones KJ, Hill WL: Auditory perception of hawks and owls for passerine alarm calls. Ethology 2001, 107(8):717-726.
  • [50]Ferguson-Lees J, Christie DA: Raptors of the world. Helm, London; 2001.
  • [51]Beason RC: What can birds hear? USDA National Wildlife Research left - Staff Publications 2004.
  • [52]Zuberbühler K: Causal knowledge of predators' behaviour in wild diana monkeys. Anim Behav 2000, 59:209-220.
  • [53]Zuberbühler K, Jenny D, Bshary R: The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology 1999, 105(6):477-490.
  • [54]Taylor AM, Reby D: The contribution of source-filter theory to mammal vocal communication research. J Zool 2010, 280:221-236.
  • [55]Smith RJ, Jungers WL: Body mass in comparative primatology. J Hum Evol 1997, 32(6):523-559.
  • [56]Barelli C, Mundry R, Heistermann M, Hammerschmidt K: Cues to androgens and quality in male gibbon songs. PLoS One 2013, 8(12):e82748.
  • [57]Owren MJ, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL: The acoustic features of vowel-like grunt calls in chacma baboons (Papio cyncephalus ursinus): Implications for production processes and functions. J Acoust Soc Am 1997, 101(5):2951-2963.
  • [58]Scott-Phillips T: Animal communication: Insights from linguistic pragmatics. Anim Behav 2010, 79:e1-e4.
  • [59]Rendall D, Owren M, Ryan MJ: What do animal signals mean? Anim Behav 2009, 78:233-240.
  • [60]Schel AM, Machanda Z, Townsend SW, Zuberbuhler K, Slocombe KE: Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals. Anim Behav 2013, 86(5):955-965.
  • [61]Simonyan K: The laryngeal motor cortex: Its organization and connectivity. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2014, 28:15-21.
  • [62]Lemasson A, Ouattara K, Zuberbühler K: Social learning of vocal structure in a nonhuman primate? BMC Evol Biol 2011, 11:362.
  • [63]Ethnologue: Languages of the world. Seventeenth edition. SIL International, Dallas, Texas; 2013.
  • [64]Koda H, Oyakawa C, Kato A, Masataka N: Experimental evidence for the volitional control of vocal production in an immature gibbon. Behaviour 2007, 144:681-692.
  • [65]Calcagno JM, Fuentes A: What makes us human? answers from evolutionary anthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology 2012, 21:182-194.
  • [66]Burling R: The talking ape. Oxford University Press, New York; 2005.
  • [67]Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL: Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain Lang 2010, 115(1):92-100.
  • [68]Hammerschmidt K, Fischer J: The vocal repertoire of Barbary macaques: a quantitative analysis of a graded system. Ethology 1998, 104:203-216.
  • [69]Fischer J, Metz M, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM: Baboon responses to graded bark variants. Anim Behav 2001, 61:925-931.
  • [70]Rendall D, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Owren MJ: The meaning and function of grunt variants in baboons. Anim Behav 1999, 57:583-592.
  • [71]Fischer J: Barbary macaques categorize shrill barks into two call types. Anim Behav 1998, 55:799-807.
  • [72]Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM: How vervet monkeys perceive their grunts: field playback experiments. Anim Behav 1982, 30(3):739-751.
  • [73]Brockelman WY, Reichard U, Treesucon U, Raemaekers JJ: Dispersal, pair formation and social structure in gibbons (Hylobates lar). BES 1998, 42:329-339.
  • [74]Savini T, Boesch C, Reichard UH: Varying ecological quality influences the probability of polyandry in white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar). Biotropica 2009, 41:503-513.
  • [75]Ey E, Hammerschmidt K, Seyfarth RM, Fischer J: Age- and sex-related variations in clear calls of papio ursinus. Int J Primatol 2007, 28:947-960.
  • [76]Owren MJ, Linker CD: Some analysis techniques that may be useful to acoustic primatologists. In Current topics in primate vocal communication. Edited by Zimmermann E, Newman JD, Jürgens U. Plenum, NY; 1995.
  • [77]Clarke E, Reichard, UH, Zuberbühler, K: Acoustic measures of gibbon (Hylobates lar) hoo calls in different contexts. LabArchives 2015, http://dx.doi.org/10.6070/H49C6VCK.
  文献评价指标  
  下载次数:8次 浏览次数:2次